Did Infections Caused by World War I Contribute to Causing World War II?
How many of those who recovered from WWI-associated infections had residual neurological impairments that increased their risk for violence?
This year, 2018, will be the 100-year anniversary of the end of World War I (WWI), which occurred at 11 AM on November 11, 1918. Wars or any other form of violence never stem from a single cause; there are many contributing factors. Unfortunately, deaths from violence do not necessarily end when wars end; in fact, "more Americans have died from guns in the United States since 1968 than on battlefields of all the wars in American history."1 The scientific capability to understand and prevent violence lags behind the technical capability of weapon development. If this trend continues, how many violent individuals, groups, or countries will have access to highly sophisticated weapons of mass destruction? By looking at past events throughout history, individuals can better understand the factors that contributed to violent actions and events, and then they can use that knowledge to reduce those factors, and thus, prevent future catastrophes.
There are many motivators that can lead to wars; common motivators include economic or territorial gain, religious conflicts, nationalism, racism, revenge, civil or revolutionary reasons, or for defensive purposes.2 Certain brain impairments increase the risk of violence; for example, injury to the right ventromedial prefrontal cortex impairs empathy, and injury to the orbital frontal cortex and other areas reduces impulse control.3 If either or both sides of a conflict are excessively influenced by individuals with brain impairments associated with violence, this may increase the risk of violence and war. However, when both sides of a conflict are influenced by individuals who are high-functioning with intact mental capabilities, the futility of war can more easily be seen, and thus, more readily avoided; a good example of this was the peaceful resolution of the Cuban missile crisis.
Warfare environments encourage the proliferation of infectious diseases. The movement of soldiers into regions where they lack immunity to local pathogens, crowding, malnutrition, stress, and deprivation during a war are all conducive to serious infectious and emerging diseases in soldiers, prisoners, refugees, as well as the general population. In fact, often, epidemics persist and expand even after a war ends. Examples of this include: the plague of Athens that occurred during the Peloponnesian War in 429 BC, bubonic plague that spread from the Venetians escaping Kaffa in 1347, the first appearance of syphilis in 1494/1495 that sprung up either from French troops returning from Naples or Columbus returning from the New World, typhus which followed Napoleon's retreat from Moscow in 1812, typhus and malaria which spread during the Korean War, cerebral malaria among Vietnam veterans, and Mycoplasma infections which sprung up in Gulf War veterans and their family members.4
During WWI, many infections, including vector-borne diseases, were identified as causing mortality, morbidity, and residual impairments.
WWI-related infections such as trench foot, trench fever (caused by louse-borne Rickettsia quintana, subsequently called Bartonella quintana), a range of helminths, intestinal parasites (including Ascaris, Trichuris, Capillaria and Taenia spp.), typhus, cholera, tuberculosis, typhoid fever, dysentery, scarlet fever, diphtheria, measles, whooping cough, and smallpox were not uncommon.5 Rape, prostitution, and sexual indiscretion that often occur during wars encourage the proliferation of venereal diseases. As the war spread beyond Europe, many soldiers were infected with malaria and other exotic diseases. Although Lyme and other tick-borne diseases were not recognized at the time, they probably added to the list of WWI infectious diseases as well. Nearly 5 million had encephalitis lethargica during that time, a third of whom died; most of the survivors had significant neurological impairments. In Germany, 800,000 died from tuberculosis between 1914 and 1920. Typhus—the greatest killer of mankind—reemerged in 1917, killed 3 million individuals (mainly in refugee camps), and was epidemic throughout the 1920s. Countless others became infected and died in prisoner-of-war and refugee camps during and following WWI.6 7 Towards the end of WWI, the Spanish Influenza epidemic expanded and went on to infect 500 million—one-fifth of the world’s population—and kill 50 million (a death toll higher than that caused by the Black Death in the Middle Ages). Furthermore, the Spanish influenza epidemic killed almost three times more individuals than the
This raises an important question: How many of the 450 million who recovered from Spanish flu or other WWI-associated infections had residual neurological impairments that increased their risk for violence?
Congenital infections during and following the war had the potential to adversely impact the developing nervous system and added to the number of those with residual impairments from these infections. Some of the infections seen in WWI are recognized to cause aggressiveness in a certain percentage of those infected. In fact, there is speculation that both Joseph Stalin and Adolf Hitler had syphilis.8, 9, 10, 11 Debate still exists regarding whether Hitler had been impaired by Spanish flu, syphilis, encephalitis lethargica, Lyme disease, and/or poison gas exposure.12 The well-documented presence of Parkinsonism symptoms in Hitler is suggestive he may have experienced post-encephalitic Parkinsonism from Spanish flu or encephalitis lethargica exposure.13 Therefore, it is quite possible that infectious disease-related impairments, fostered by WWI, may have impacted the behavior and judgment of Hitler, as well as many others on both sides of the conflict, and may have been a contributing factor in intensifying the conflicts of WWII.
To those working with individuals with mental symptoms associated with chronic and prior infections, it is clear there are some who show irritability, intolerance, poor impulse control, paranoia, predatory aggression, and other risk factors associated with aggressiveness after they become infected.14 Although we cannot absolutely prove nor disprove that WWI infections contributed to causing WWII, if close to one-fifth of the world’s population had infections with possible neurological sequelae it raises the likelihood of such an association.
Internal server error